Friday, July 26, 2024

Section 36–6 Neurology of vision

(Nerve impulse / Contrast enhancement / Feature detectors)

 

In this section, Feynman discusses nerve impulse and experiments on the nerve fibers of horseshoe crab (related to the principle of contrast enhancement) and frog (feature detectors).

 

1. Nerve impulse:

First of all, we must appreciate what kind of information can come along nerves. A nerve carries a kind of disturbance which has an electrical effect that is easy to detect, a kind of wavelike disturbance which runs down the nerve and produces an effect at the other end: a long piece of the nerve cell, called the axon, carries the information along, and a certain kind of impulse, called a “spike,” goes along if it is excited at one end (Feynman et al., 1963, p. 36–9).”

 

The “spike” (disturbance) is also known as “nerve impulse” or “action potential” that moves along a nerve fiber. nerve impulse is an electrical phenomenon that occurs due to differences in sodium ion-concentrations across the membrane of a neuron (Hodgkin, 1964). In other words, the nerve impulse is a sudden reversal of the electrical charge across the membrane of the neuron. The Nobel Prize in Physiology or Medicine 1963 was awarded to Alan Hodgkin and Andrew Huxley for their discoveries concerning the ionic mechanisms involved in excitation and inhibition in the peripheral and central portions of the nerve cell membrane. The electrochemical nature of the nerve impulse was investigated by Hodgkin and Huxley in their studies of squid giant axons.


“When one spike goes down the nerve, another cannot immediately follow. All the spikes are of the same size, so it is not that we get higher spikes when the thing is more strongly excited, but that we get more spikes per second. The size of the spike is determined by the fiber (Feynman et al., 1963, p. 36–9).”

 

Perhaps Feynman could have included the “all or none principle,” which means neurons will either transmit a nerve impulse over the synapse to the next neuron completely or not at all. In short, the nerve impulse is not dependent on the strength of the stimuli, but it is generated when the initial threshold is met (see below). Specifically, the membrane potential of a neuron is about -70 mV in the resting state (Patton & Thibodeau, 2018). If a stimulus is above the threshold potential, the membrane depolarises, i.e., sodium-gated ion channels open and allow sodium ions to move into the axon, making the membrane potential more positive. This principle underscores the binary nature of action potential propagation: a neuron either fires a full-sized action potential or it does not fire at all.

 

Source: Patton & Thibodeau, 2018

2. Contrast enhancement:

We can see, perhaps, if we think about it awhile, that this is a device to enhance contrast at the edges of objects, because if a part of the scene is light and a part is black, then the ommatidia in the lighted area give impulses that are inhibited by all the other light in the neighborhood, so it is relatively weak (Feynman et al., 1963, p. 36–10).”

 

Historically, Haldan Keffer Hartline (1949) identified the lateral inhibition* phenomenon in his experiment on the compound eye of the horseshoe crab. Furthermore, he showed that its photoreceptor cells are interconnected and communicated via the mechanism of lateral inhibition: the ability of excited neurons to reduce the activity of neighboring neurons. In essence, lateral inhibition of neurons can help to enhance contrast at the edge of the objects perceived by the eye. Interestingly, Hartline discovered the phenomenon accidentally when he turned on the room lights and found that the neural response of a horseshoe crab eye decreased. Hartline was awarded the 1967 Nobel Prize in Medicine or Physiology for his research in the neurophysiological mechanisms of vision.

 

*The lateral inhibition phenomenon, which occurs in the retina was first recognized by Ernst Mach, in the year 1866.


“Figure 36–11(a) shows the compound eye of the horseshoe crab; it is not very much of an eye, it has only about a thousand ommatidia. Figure 36–11(b) is a cross section through the system; one can see the ommatidia, with the nerve fibers that run out of them and go into the brain. But note that even in a horseshoe crab there are little interconnections. They are much less elaborate than in the human eye, and it gives us a chance to study a simpler example (Feynman et al., 1963, p. 36–10).”

 

It is worth mentioning that an ommatidium is not an independent eye but a component of a compound eye; each ommatidium functions as a part of the whole, providing information about a small segment of the visual field. In addition, horseshoe crab (or Limulus), has three different types of eyes: a pair of image-forming lateral compound eyes, a pair of median ocelli, and three pair of larval eyes—lateral, median, and ventral (Tanacredi et al., 2009). However, horseshoe crabs are not really crabs (crustaceans), but are more closely related to spiders and scorpions. More important, horseshoe crabs are “living fossils,” i.e., they have existed nearly unchanged for at least 445 million years, even before dinosaurs existed. Hartline used the horseshoe crab as his first experimental model because of its compound eye, easy-to-isolate optic nerve, and individual photoreceptor cells help to understand the principle of contrast enhancement.

 

3. Frog’s feature detectors:

The most recent picture of the operation of the frog’s eye is the following. One can find four different kinds of optic nerve fibers, in the sense that there are four different kinds of responses. These experiments were not done by shining on-and-off impulses of light, because that is not what a frog sees. A frog just sits there and his eyes never move, unless the lily pad is flopping back and forth, and in that case his eyes wobble just right so that the image stays put. He does not turn his eyes. If anything moves in his field of vision, like a little bug (he has to be able to see something small moving in the fixed background), it turns out that there are four different kinds of fibers which discharge, whose properties are summarized in Table 36–1 (Feynman et al., 1963, p. 36–11).”

 

Feynman mentions that there are four different kinds of nerve fibers, but summarizes five types of neural responses as Sustained edge detection, Convex edge detection, Changing contrast detection, Dimming detection, and Darkness detection. He adds that the neural responses seem to be rather complicated to classify, however, Smoorenburg (1987) identified the four responses as Sustained Contrast detector, Convexity detector, Moving Edge detector, and Net-Dimming detector. Interestingly, toads, a sub-classification of frogs, have only three primary types of responses in their visual processing system: (1) Sustained edge detector, (2) Convexity detector, and (3) Moving edge detector. While frogs exhibit four types of neural responses, including dimming detectors, the toads’ system has evolved for their specific ecological needs.

 

Now these responses seem to be rather complicated to classify, and we might wonder whether perhaps the experiments are being misinterpreted. But it is very interesting that these same classes are very clearly separated in the anatomy of the frog! By other measurements, after these responses had been classified (afterwards, that is what is important about this), it was discovered that the speed of the signals on the different fibers was not the same, so here was another, independent way to check which kind of a fiber we have found! (Feynman et al., 1963, p. 36–12).”

 

According to Lettvin and his collaborators’ (1959) study, there are four types of nerve fibers or feature detectors in the frog’s eyes. The four types of feature detectors can be classified as follows: (1) Contrast (Sustained Edge Detector): respond to the presence of edges in the visual field and continue to signal as long as the edge is visible. (2) Convexity (Convex Edge Detector): sensitive to convex shapes, which might resemble preys. (3) Changing contrast (Change-in-Contrast Detector): fire in response to moving edges or changes in contrast but do not maintain the signal if the object is stationary. (4) Darkness (Dimming Detector): increase their firing rate when the light intensity decreases. They also suggested the idea of “bug detectors” that respond to small, dark, and moving objects. However, the frog’s eye behaves like a quantum sensor because it is possible to use a quantum light source to show that the frog’s light-sensitive rod cells can detect single photon (Phan et al., 2014).

 

Review Questions:

1. How would you explain the neural impulse using the all-or-none principle?

2. How would you relate the lateral inhibition phenomenon to the principle of contrast enhancement?

3. How would you classify the frog’s feature detectors (or types of neural response in nerve fibers)?

 

The moral of the lesson: The lateral inhibition (or contrast enhancement) in horseshoe crabs and feature detections (three, four, or five) in frogs illustrate that brain-eye systems and neural responses are dependent on how environments evolve.

 

References:

1. Ewert, J. P. (1974). The neural basis of visually guided behavior. Scientific American230(3), 34-43.

2. Feynman, R. P., Leighton, R. B., & Sands, M. (1963). The Feynman Lectures on Physics, Vol I: Mainly mechanics, radiation, and heat. Reading, MA: Addison-Wesley.

3. Hartline, H. K. (1949). Inhibition of activity of visual receptors by illuminating nearby retinal areas in the Limulus eye (abstract), Federation Proceedings, 8, 69.

4. Hodgkin, A. L. (1964). The ionic basis of nervous conduction. Science145(3637), 1148-1154.

5. Lettvin, J. Y., Maturana, H. R., McCulloch, W. S., & Pitts, W. H. (1959). What the frog's eye tells the frog's brain. Proceedings of the IRE47(11), 1940-1951.

6. Patton, K. T., & Thibodeau, G. A. (2018). Anthony's Textbook of Anatomy & Physiology-E-Book: Anthony's Textbook of Anatomy & Physiology-E-Book. Elsevier Health Sciences.

7. Phan, N. M., Cheng, M. F., Bessarab, D. A., & Krivitsky, L. A. (2014). Interaction of fixed number of photons with retinal rod cells. Physical Review Letters112(21), 213601.

8. Smoorenburg, G. F. (1987). Discussion of physiological correlates of speech perception. In The psychophysics of speech perception (pp. 393-399). Dordrecht: Springer Netherlands.

9. Tanacredi, J. T., Botton, M. L., & Smith, D. R. (Eds.). (2009). Biology and conservation of horseshoe crabs. New York: Springer.

Friday, July 5, 2024

Section 36–5 Other eyes

(Sexual selection / Convergent evolution / Extreme adaptation)

 

In this section, Feynman discusses the evolution of sexual selection that influences peacock’s color vision and convergent evolution through the development of the octopus’s eye, as well as mentions the giant squid's enormous eyes, but it could be related to extreme adaptation. Simply phrased, the section is about the eyes of peacock, octopus, and giant squid that are related to sexual selection, convergent evolution, and extreme adaptation.

 

1. Peacock’s eye:

Fish, butterflies, birds, and reptiles can see color, but it is believed that most mammals cannot. The primates can see color. The birds certainly see color, and that accounts for the colors of birds. There would be no point in having such brilliantly colored males if the females could not notice it! That is, the evolution of the sexual “whatever it is” that the birds have is a result of the female being able to see color (Feynman et al., 1963, p. 36–9).”

 

Feynman says that the evolution of the sexual “whatever it is” that the birds have is a result of the female being able to see color. On the other hand, Darwin (1859) writes: “when the males and females of any animal have the same general habits… but differ in structure, color, or ornament, such differences have been mainly caused by sexual selection (p. 89).” Thus, one may explain that the evolution of sexual selection influences the color vision in animals. In other words, many animals can see color, which plays a crucial role in sexual selection. In peacocks, for example, the vivid colors of males have evolved to attract females, indicating that female birds’ ability to see color drives the development of bright plumage.

 

“So next time we look at a peacock and think of what a brilliant display of gorgeous color it is, and how delicate all the colors are, and what a wonderful aesthetic sense it takes to appreciate all that, we should not compliment the peacock, but should compliment the visual acuity and aesthetic sense of the peahen, because that is what has generated the beautiful scene! (Feynman et al., 1963, p. 36–9).”

 

In his lectures titled QED delivered at UCLA for the public, Feynman explains the iridescent feathers as a phenomenon of colors produced by the partial reflection of white light by two surfaces. Furthermore, he adds, “[P]erhaps you have wondered how the brilliant colors of hummingbirds and peacocks are produced. Now you know. How those brilliant colors evolved is also an interesting question. When we admire a peacock, we should give credit to the generations of lackluster females for being selective about their mates (p. 35).” However, in a letter to Asa Gray, Darwin (1860) writes, “the sight of a feather in a peacock’s tail, whenever I gaze at it, makes me sick!” Darwin thought that the peacock’s feather did not support his theory of evolution because the feather could make it difficult for the peacock to escape predators.

 

2. Octopus’ eye:

It is very interesting that, besides the development of its brain and its reactions and so on, which are rather good for an invertebrate, it has also developed, independently, a different eye. It is not a compound eye or an eye spot—it has a cornea, it has lids, it has an iris, it has a lens, it has two regions of water, it has a retina behind. It is essentially the same as the eye of the vertebrates! It is a remarkable example of a coincidence in evolution where nature has twice discovered the same solution to a problem, with one slight improvement (Feynman et al., 1963, p. 36–9).”

 

Convergent evolution refers to the evolution of very similar traits independently in different organisms that are not closely related (Roberts, 1986). In short, Feynman describes convergent evolution through the development of the octopus’ eye.  The octopus’ eye is an example of convergent evolution, where similar eye structures have independently evolved in both vertebrates and invertebrate (or cephalopods). Despite being an invertebrate, the octopus has developed a pair of eyes with a cornea, iris, lens, and retina, similar to vertebrate eyes. However, the claim “nature has twice discovered the same solution to a problem” is a simplification. While convergent evolution has led to similar structures, the developmental pathways and genetic underpinnings of the eye differ significantly.

 

“In the octopus it also turns out, amazingly, that the retina is a piece of the brain that has come out in the same way in its embryonic development as is true for vertebrates, but the interesting thing which is different is that the cells which are sensitive to light are on the inside, and the cells which do the calculation are in back of them, rather than “inside out,” as in our eye (Feynman et al., 1963, p. 36–9).”

 

Despite the similarities in function, the eyes of octopus and human differ in their developmental origins, structural organization, and focusing mechanisms. There are at least three important differences: (1) Retina orientation: The retina of the octopus is directly oriented towards incoming light and thus, the photoreceptor cells are in front of the nerve fibers. On the contrary, the human retina is oriented in the opposite direction as shown below. (2) Blind spots: The octopus’ eye does not have a blind spot because the nerve fibers are behind the photoreceptors; human eye has a blind spot due to the optic nerve exiting the back of the eye, where there are no photoreceptors. (3) Photoreceptors: Octopus typically have one type of photoreceptor, and some species are sensitive to polarized light, which aids in navigation and detecting prey. On the other hand, humans have three types of cones sensitive to red, green, and blue.

Source: Roberts, 1986



3. Squid’s eye:

“The biggest eyes in the world are those of the giant squid; they have been found up to 15 inches in diameter! (Feynman et al., 1963, p. 36–9).”

 

Feynman only mentions that the biggest eyes in the world are those of the giant squid that have been found up to 15 inches in diameter. However, it is worthwhile to include the term extreme adaptation because the giant squid has adapted to living in the deep sea by developing a large body size and enormous eyes that allow it to see in low-light conditions. Specifically, the Atlantic giant squid (Architeuthis dux) has the largest eyes in the world. It has been estimated that the record example from Thimble Tickle Bay, Newfoundland, Canada, in 1878 had eyes measuring 40 cm (15.75 in) in diameter (Breverton, 2013). Interestingly, every octopus has eight arms – limbs with suckers dotted all the way along, whereas a squid has not just eight arms but also two tentacles, with suckers just at the end, which it uses to hunt fish and shrimp.

 

Octopus and squid eyes share some similarities due to their common cephalopod lineage, but have distinct differences reflective of their specific adaptations and lifestyles. Similarities: Both octopus and squid eyes function like a camera, with a single lens focusing light onto a retina, and have pupils that can change shape. Furthermore, neither the octopus nor the squid eye has a blind spot because the optic nerve does not pass through the retina. Differences: Squid often have larger eyes relative to their body size compared to octopuses. For example, the colossal squid has some of the largest eyes in the animal kingdom, which helps detect faint light in the dark ocean depths. These differences manifest in aspects like habitat-specific adaptations, eye size, light sensitivity, and behavioral use of vision.

 

Review Questions:

1. How would you explain the evolution of sexual selection influences the color vision of peacocks?

2. How would you explain the concept of convergent evolution through the octopus’ eye and human eyes?

3. How would you explain the biggest eyes in the world are those of the giant squid due to extreme adaptation?

 

The moral of the lesson: this section highlights how sexual selection influences color vision in animals, illustrates convergent evolution through the development of the octopus eye, and demonstrates extreme adaptation of the giant squid's enormous eyes.

 

References:

1. Breverton, T. (2013). Breverton's Nautical Curiosities: A Book of the Sea. London: Quercus.

2. Darwin, C. (1859). On the origin of species: facsimile of the first edition.

3. Darwin, C. (1993). The correspondence of Charles Darwin. 8. 1860 (Vol. 8). Cambridge University Press.

4. Feynman, R. P. (1985). QED: The strange theory of light and matter. Princeton: Princeton University Press.

5. Feynman, R. P., Leighton, R. B., & Sands, M. (1963). The Feynman Lectures on Physics, Vol I: Mainly mechanics, radiation, and heat. Reading, MA: Addison-Wesley.

6. Roberts, M. B. V. (1986). Biology: a functional approach. Cheltenham: Nelson Thornes.