(Dark-adapted vision / Purkinje effect / Peripheral vision)
In
this section, Feynman discusses the dark-adapted vision, Purkinje effect (related to mesopic vision), and
peripheral vision that depend on the intensity of light.
1. Dark-adapted vision:
“If the intensity of the
light is very low, the things that we see have no color. It is
known that this dark-adapted vision is almost entirely due to the rods,
while the vision in bright light is due to the cones. As a result, there are a
number of phenomena that we can easily appreciate because of this transfer of
function from the cones and rods together, to just the rods (Feynman et al., 1963, p. 35–2).”
It is potentially misleading to say “if the intensity of the light is
very low, the things that we see have no color.” In very low-light
conditions, we rely on our rod cells, which are highly sensitive to low levels
of light, to see in shades of gray (or grey). While rod cells do not provide
detailed color information, they allow us to distinguish different levels of
brightness and perceive objects in a grayscale view. On the other hand, gray is a
color because it can be achieved by adding and adjusting the intensity of
red, green, and blue light (see below). However, rod cells may take about 20-30 minutes
for the human eye to become fully dark-adapted, that is, allowing the rod cells
to reach their maximum sensitivity in the dark night.
Dark-adapted vision primarily
refers to scotopic vision, which is the vision that occurs in very low-light
conditions. In his autobiography, Feynman (1997) mentions that he could see
colors during an atomic explosion in the dark: “… a big ball of orange, the center
that was so bright, becomes a ball of orange that starts to rise and billow a
little bit and get a little black around the edges, and then you see it's a big
ball of smoke with flashes on the inside, with the heat of the fire going
outwards. All this took about one minute. It was a series from bright to dark,
and I had seen it. I am about the only guy who actually looked at the damn
thing--the first Trinity test (p. 134).” In a sense, this is another reason why it is incorrect
to say that the things that we see have no color in the dark. (Interestingly, some
claim to achieve beatific vision during the dark night.)
2. Purkinje effect:
“It turns out that the rods
see better toward the blue than the cones do, and the cones can see, for
example, deep red light, while the rods find that absolutely impossible to see.
So red light is black so far as the rods are concerned. Thus two pieces of
colored paper, say blue and red, in which the red might be even brighter than
the blue in good light, will, in the dark, appear completely reversed. It is a
very striking effect. If we are in the dark and can find a magazine or
something that has colors and, before we know for sure what the colors are, we
judge the lighter and darker areas, and if we then carry the magazine into the
light, we may see this very remarkable shift between which was the brightest
color and which was not. The phenomenon is called the Purkinje effect (Feynman et al., 1963, p. 35–2).”
Historically, Jan Evangelista Purkinje observed that his favorite flower
appeared bright red in the afternoon, but it became bluish-red in the evening. We
can explain Purkinje
effect as follows: (1) Lighting conditions: It pertains to the shift in
perceived colors under different lighting conditions, specifically during
transitions from bright light (photopic vision) to low-light conditions
(mesopic vision). (2) Color perception: In bright light, shorter wavelengths
dominate our perception, making blue and green hues more vibrant, whereas in
low-light conditions, longer wavelengths become more prominent. (3) cone cells:
The effect occurs due to the different sensitivity of S-cones*, M-cones, and L-cones under
different lighting conditions. In short, Purkinje
effect may refer to red objects that appear darker under dim lighting
conditions compared to green objects due to the reduced sensitivity of the cone
cells to red wavelengths (See below).
*Humans have three types of cone cells in the retina, each sensitive to different wavelengths of light: short-wavelength cones (S-cones) are sensitive to blue light, medium-wavelength cones (M-cones) are sensitive to green light, and long-wavelength cones (L-cones) are sensitive to red light.
In Fig. 35–3, the dashed curve represents the sensitivity of the eye in the dark, i.e., using the rods, while the solid curve represents it in the light. We see that the peak sensitivity of the rods is in the green region and that of the cones is more in the yellow region. If there is a red-colored page (red is about 650 μm) we can see it if it is brightly lighted, but in the dark it is almost invisible (Feynman et al., 1963, p. 35–2).”
Feynman initially explains the phenomenon using two pieces of colored paper, blue and red, but elaborates the spectral sensitivity of the eye involving the cones that is more in the yellow region. However, it could be explained in terms of photopic vision and scotopic vision: (1) Photopic vision: When our eyes are exposed to bright light conditions, such as well-lit indoor environments, they are in a state of photopic vision. In photopic vision, our eyes (cones) are most sensitive to the yellow-green part of the spectrum, about 555 nanometers (nm) wavelength. (2) Scotopic vision: When our eyes are exposed to very low-light conditions, they gradually adapt to scotopic vision, which is highly sensitive to dim light. In scotopic vision, our eyes (rods) are most sensitive to about 505 nm wavelength, which corresponds to the color green on the visible spectrum.
3. Peripheral vision:
“Another interesting
phenomenon is that the periphery of the retina is very sensitive to motion.
Although we cannot see very well from the corner of our eye, if a little bug
moves and we do not expect anything to be moving over there, we are immediately
sensitive to it. We are all “wired up” to look for something jiggling to the
side of the field (Feynman et al., 1963, p. 35–3).”
The phenomenon described by
Feynman is related to the distribution of rod cells in the human retina and how
it affects our peripheral vision. We may define peripheral vision (or side
vision) as follows: 1. Field of vision: It covers a wider field of view
compared to central vision. 2. Sensitivity to motion: It is highly sensitive to
motion and is crucial for detecting movement in the surrounding environment due
to a higher density of rod cells. 3. Lower acuity and color perception: It has
lower visual acuity and reduced color perception compared to central vision due
to a lower density of cone cells. Thus, the periphery of the retina is better
suited for detecting motion and objects in dim lighting conditions.
No comments:
Post a Comment