(Grassmann’s additive law / Additive color model / Fundamental primary colors)
In this section, Feynman discusses the Grassmann’s
additive law of color mixing, additive
color model, and fundamental
primary colors. This section could be titled
as Young-Helmholtz-Maxwell trichromatic theory, however, Feynman mentions
Young-Helmholtz theory in the next section that also discusses color blindness.
Interestingly, in his autobiography, What do you care
what other people think?, Feynman (1988) writes: “When I see equations, I
see letters in colors - I don't know why. As I'm
talking, I see vague pictures of Bessel functions from Jahnke and Emde's book,
with light-tan j's, slightly violet-bluish n's, and dark brown x's
flying around. And I wonder what the hell it must look like
to the students (p. 59).” This neurological phenomenon is known as synaesthesia
and specifically, Feynman’s description of seeing the equations and letters in
colors is sometimes called "grapheme-color" synaesthesia (Henshaw, 2012).
1. Grassmann’s additive law:
“Here is one of the great laws of color: if two spectral distributions are indistinguishable, and we add to each one a certain light, say Z (if we write X+Z, this means that we shine both lights on the same patch), and then we take Y and add the same amount of the same other light, Z, the new mixtures are also indistinguishable: X+Z = Y+Z (Feynman et al., 1963, p. 35–5).”
The law states that if two spectral distributions (or different lights) are indistinguishable to our eyes under a certain condition, and we add the same additional light (Z) to both of them, the resulting mixtures will still be indistinguishable. Similarly, we may state Grassmann’s (1853) additive law of color mixture as follows: if two different lights or colors are indistinguishable (metamers*), we can add another color to them and they will still be metamers. One may clarify that many different combinations of wavelengths can result in indistinguishable color sensations, that is, human eyes are not very precise light detectors. Furthermore, there are different types of metamers, e.g., material metamers refer to two different surface reflectance curves are perceived as the same color when each is viewed using the same light source. In essence, Grassmann’s additive law is an idealization that assumes human eyes are a linear system of color perception (Oleari, 2015).
* Metamers: Metameric lights are lights that though of dissimilar
spectral radiation are seen as the same by the observer (Shevell, 2003).
“In fact, it turns out, and it is very important and interesting, that
this matching of the color of lights is not dependent upon the characteristics
of the eye at the moment of observation: we know that if we look for a long
time at a bright red surface, or a bright red light, and then look at a white
paper, it looks greenish, and other colors are also distorted by our having
looked so long at the bright red (Feynman et al., 1963, p. 35–5).”
The
phenomenon Feynman described is known as color afterimage (see below),
which occurs due to the way our eyes and brain process visual information. When
you stare at a red surface or light for an extended period, the cone cells in
your eyes, which are responsible for color vision, become desensitized to the
specific wavelengths of light associated with red. When you then shift your
gaze to a white surface, your eyes continue to send signals to your brain as if
they are still seeing red. Since the red-sensitive cone cells are temporarily
less responsive, the signals received by your brain are weaker than they would
be under normal conditions. This effect is a result of the opponent-process
theory of color vision, which suggests that color perception is based on
the opposition between pairs of colors: red versus green, blue versus yellow,
and black versus white (Triedman, 2015).
In the next section, Feynman mentions the term afterimage, but it does
not seem necessary.
![]() |
Source: Sight and Visual Perception - Course Hero |
2. Additive color model:
“The
second principle of color mixing of lights is this: any color at all
can be made from three different colors, in our case, red, green, and blue
lights. By suitably mixing the three together we can make anything at all, as
we demonstrated with our two examples… Then any color could be made by certain
amounts of these three: say an amount a of color A, an amount b of
color B, and an amount c of color C makes X: X =
aA+bB+cC (Feynman et
al., 1963, p. 35–5).”
The
second principle is sometimes known as the principle of additive color mixing,
often used to describe how different colors of light combine to form new
colors. This principle is associated with the RGB (Red, Green, Blue) additive color
model (see below), where colors are created by combining different intensities
of red, green, and blue light. It is an
approximation model due to its reliance on a linear relationship between
primary colors and their combinations, which simplifies the complexities
inherent in color perception and light interaction. It
forms the basis for understanding how different colored lights combine and
interact in various display technologies, such as computer monitors, TVs, and
stage lighting. The model is based on standard observers,
but color perception varies from person to person and within the same observers
during their lifetimes due to aging.
Source: Additive & Subtractive Color Models > DINFOS Pavilion > Article
“In
elementary books they are said to be red, green, and blue, but that is merely
because with these a wider range of colors is available
without minus signs for some of the combinations (Feynman et al., 1963, p. 35–6).”
In elementary schools, students usually learn red-yellow-blue (RYB) color system when
painting in an art class. On the other hand, the cyan, magenta, and yellow
(CMY) color system is preferred for painting because it is more cost-effective
to provide a wider range of colors. These are known as subtractive color
models because colors are created by subtracting certain
wavelengths of light, i.e., light is either reflected or absorbed by an object
depending on its pigmentation (or composition of fabric dyes).
However, it is easier to teach kids using red-yellow-blue
color system because the words "cyan" and "magenta" are
more difficult for them.
3.
Fundamental primary colors:
“Now a question is, what are the correct primary colors to use? There is
no such thing as “the” correct primary colors for the mixing of lights.
There may be, for practical purposes, three paints that are more useful than
others for getting a greater variety of mixed pigments, but we are not
discussing that matter now. Any three differently colored lights
whatsoever can always be mixed in the correct proportion to
produce any color whatsoever… Every set of three primaries requires
negative amounts for some colors, and therefore there is no unique way to
define a primary (Feynman et
al., 1963, p. 35–6).”
There is no universally agreed upon set of fundamental primary colors partly because different color models are based on different principles of color mixing – additive or subtractive. In the RGB (Red, Green, Blue) color model commonly used in electronic displays like monitors and TVs, red, green, and blue are considered the primary colors. In the CMYK (Cyan, Magenta, Yellow, Black) color model used in color printing, cyan, magenta, and yellow are considered the primary colors. Furthermore, there are different types of RGB system, such as Standard RGB and Wide-gamut RGB, where each has its own specifications, color gamuts, and intended uses. Interestingly, imaginary primary colors are used to generate and quantify all visible colors that can be perceived by the human eyes.
Historically, Young proposed that the human eye could distinguish three primary colors, namely, red, green, and violet. Next, Helmholtz classified the cone receptors as short (violet), middle (green), and long (red); he recognized the difference between experiments done by mixing pigments and light beams. It is worth mentioning that Maxwell's contributions to the theory of color are not limited to the following: (1) Primary colors: Maxwell's (1857) experiments supported the idea that red, green, and blue are a better set of primary colors compared to the traditional set of red, yellow, and blue. (2) Distinction between Paints and Light beams: Maxwell distinguished between the primary colors used for mixing light beams and those used in painting. (3) Color attributes: Maxwell defined hue (spectral color determined by wavelength), tint (degree of color saturation), and shade (intensity of illumination). The theory described by Feynman could be called Young-Helmholtz-Maxwell trichromatic theory, but there are other contributors, such as Newton, Grassmann, and Schrödinger*.
*Note: According to a recent
research study (Bujack et al., 2022), there
is a significant error in the 3D mathematical space developed by Erwin
Schrödinger and others to describe how human eye distinguishes one color from
another.
No comments:
Post a Comment