(Dotted Triangle / Horseshoe shaped area / Spectral sensitivity curves)
In this section, Feynman discusses the dotted
triangle and horseshoe
shaped area of a chromaticity
diagram as well as spectral sensitivity curves.
Alternatively,
the section could be titled “The CIE chromaticity
diagram (1931 version)” because it was based on CIE 1931 color space in which
CIE stands for Commission Internationale de l’Eclairage. The
chromaticity diagram is a representation of human color perception developed from
the experimental results conducted by John Guild and David Wright. In addition,
the CIE defined
the standard observer in 1931 using a 2° field of view, which represents
an average human’s chromatic response. In 1964, the CIE proposed some
improvements, e.g., they defined an additional standard observer using a 10°
field of view.
1. Dotted Triangle:
“If any one color is
represented by Eq. (35.4), we can plot it as a vector in space by
plotting along three axes the amounts a, b, and c, and then a
certain color is a point. If another color is a′, b′, c′,
that color is located somewhere else. The sum of the two, as we know, is the
color which comes from adding these as vectors… If we use a blue and a green
and a red, as primaries, we see that all the colors that we can make with
positive coefficients are inside the dotted triangle, which contains
almost all of the colors that we can ever see… (Feynman et al., 1963, p. 35–6).”
The
dotted triangle (or triangular region) contains the colors that can be produced
by mixing three primary colors. Each primary color is typically located at a corner
of the triangle. The color equation can be represented by C = aR +
bG + cB, in
which C is the
color to be matched, R, G,
B are the chosen primary colors, whereas
the chromaticity coordinates a, b, c are the amount of each primary color.
One may adjust the brightness of the color C by multiplying
the coordinates a, b, c by a constant in which a + b
+ c = 1. Feynman explains that we can plot any color as a vector in
the context of the chromaticity diagram, however, the chromaticity coordinates
do not form a vector space in the formal sense. The vector space is a
mathematical structure that satisfies certain properties, such as closure under
addition and scalar multiplication, among others.
Maxwell developed a chart in the form of an equilateral
triangle, which represents the relationships between different
colors and it approaches pure white at the center. The Maxwell’s color triangle is
an idealized model that helps visualize how different colors can be combined in
varying proportions to form a wide range of hues (colors dependent on the
dominant wavelength). Essentially, any point within the triangle identifies a specific color,
and it illustrates the additive color mixing process. The interior of the triangle
displays the secondary and tertiary colors that result from combining different
proportions of the primary colors, e.g., red and green form yellow, green and
blue form cyan, whereas blue and red form magenta (see below). However, this
triangle has limitations in representing the full
complexity of human color perception, individual variations in color vision,
and the intricacies of color mixing in different contexts.
![]() |
Source: Maxwell's Triangle (appstate.edu) |
2. Horseshoe shaped area:
“…
because all the colors that we can ever see are enclosed in the oddly shaped
area bounded by the curve. Where did this area come from? Once somebody
made a very careful match of all the colors that we can see against three
special ones (Feynman et
al., 1963, p. 35–7).”
The
chromaticity diagram is a plane diagram formed by plotting one of the
chromaticity coordinates against another that shows a range of colors. Some colorists
prefer the term gamut, which means the range of colors that can be formed
by mixing different ratios of primary colors. The color gamut depicted in a chromaticity diagram,
appears in the shape of a horseshoe (instead of saying oddly shaped area).
In other words, the chromaticity coordinates of the pure
colors in the visible spectral range form a concave curve shaped like the “sole
of a shoe.” The horseshoe shaped area is a representation of
the limits of human vision and the range of colors that can be perceived by the
average human eye. Colors lying outside the horseshoe boundary are imaginary
and cannot be produced by any combination of the three primary colors within
the visible spectrum.
Historically,
scientists assumed that three primary colors could be mixed to form all colors,
but it was not achievable due to the impurity (or imperfection) of the
paints. Furthermore, the original CIE chromaticity diagram is an imperfect
system because it is unable to generate the full range of visible
(perceptually possible) colors. However, it is possible to “mix” imaginary
primary colors and quantify all colors, i.e., these non-real primary colors are
defined as lying outside the range of visible colors (see below). Specifically,
CIE 1931 XYZ color space (based on
imaginary primary colors) was developed from experimental data performed by
Wright and Guild in the 1920’s and it serves
as a reference for other color spaces. In essence, it is impossible to
create all visible colors using real primary colors (e.g., red, green, and
blue), however, all visible colors can be mixed using imaginary primary
colors that are perceptually impossible.
![]() |
Source: handprint: colormaking attributes |
3. Spectral sensitivity curves:
“An example of such experimental results for mixing three lights
together is given in Fig. 35–5. This figure shows the amount of each of
three different particular primaries, red, green and blue, which is required to
make each of the spectral colors. Red is at the left end of the spectrum,
yellow is next, and so on, all the way to blue. Notice that at some points
minus signs are necessary. It is from such data that it is possible to locate
the position of all of the colors on a chart, where the x- and
the y-coordinates are related to the amounts of the different primaries
that are used (Feynman et
al., 1963, p. 35–7).”
Fig. 35–5 is described in The
Feynman Lectures as “The color coefficients of pure spectral colors in
terms of a certain set of standard primary.” Perhaps Feynman could use the term Color Matching
Functions (CMFs) or “original Wright and Guild RGB functions,”
which are a set of mathematical functions that describe how the human eye
perceives different wavelengths of light. In short, the CMFs are the amounts of
red, green, and blue light needed to match the standardized intensity of light
of a certain wavelength. On the other hand, there are spectral sensitivity curves of three idealized
light detectors yielding the CIE tristimulus values X, Y and Z
(see below). Feynman explains that at some points minus signs are necessary for RGB CMFs in Fig.
35–5, but this is not necessary for XYZ CMFs that have positive values
for all wavelengths, which is an advantage.
![]() |
Source: CIE 1931 color space - Wikipedia |
“That is the way that the curved boundary line has been found. It is the locus of the pure spectral colors. Now any other color can be made by adding spectral lines, of course, and so we find that anything that can be produced by connecting one part of this curve to another is a color that is available in nature (Feynman et al., 1963, p. 35–7).”
The
curved boundary line in the chromaticity diagram (see below) shows three
aspects of color perception: (1) Limits of vision: It delineates the limits of
color vision that can be perceived by the human eye under normal lighting conditions.
(2) Hue and saturation: It defines the perceivable hues
and maximum saturation, while those nearer to the center are less saturated. Saturation refers to “the attribute of color
perception that expresses the degree of departure from the gray of the same
lightness (Hunter
& Harold, 1987, p. 407).” (3) Spectral
(monochromatic) locus: It represents the path of pure spectral colors at
various wavelengths (as shown below in nanometers) along the visible spectrum. In summary, the Color Matching Functions (CMFs) and
curved boundary line are closely linked, as the CMFs guide our understanding of
how the three primary colors should be mixed to replicate the perception of
colors found along the boundary curve, representing the pure spectral colors
visible to human vision.
![]() |
Source: (Rhyne, 2012) |
Review
Questions:
1.
Do you agree with Feynman that any color within the dotted triangle can be plotted as a vector in space?
2.
How would you explain the oddly shaped area in the chromaticity diagram?
3. How
would you explain the curved boundary line
in the chromaticity diagram?
The
moral of the lesson: the horseshoe-shaped area in the chromaticity
diagram contains the entire range of colors visible to the human eye, encompassing
both pure spectral colors and all the colors that result from combinations of
the three primary colors.
References:
1. Feynman, R. P., Leighton, R. B., &
Sands, M. (1963). The Feynman
Lectures on Physics, Vol I: Mainly mechanics, radiation, and
heat. Reading, MA: Addison-Wesley.
2. Hunter, R. S., & Harold, R. W. (1987). The measurement of appearance. New York: John Wiley & Sons.
3. Rhyne, T. M. (2012). Applying color theory to digital media and visualization. Boca Raton: CRC Press.
No comments:
Post a Comment